Handout_10 SV(1)

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    1/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 581

    Two-Level Fractional Factorial Designs

    Motivation

    Fractionating a Design

    The Defining Relation

    Confounding Pattern

    Design Resolution

    Catalog of Defining Relations

    Interactions

    Saturated DesignFoldover Design

    Power and Sample Size

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    2/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 582

    Two-Level Fractional Factorial Designs Motivation

    It is apparent that as k, the number of experimental factors that are

    varied in a two-level full factorial experiment, becomes larger than

    five, the number of runs required, 2k, becomes prohibitive.

    Fractional factorial designs make more efficient use of full factorialdesigns by confounding potentially unimportant pieces of

    information with important pieces of information.

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    3/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 583

    Fractionating a Design

    As the number of experimental factors increases in a two-level full

    factorial experiment, so do the number and order of interaction

    terms that are estimable in the linear model.

    For example, in a five factor experiment, there are:

    one constant

    five main effects

    ten second-order interactions

    ten third-order interactions

    five fourth-order interactions

    one fifth-order interactions

    that can be estimated. This adds up to a total of 32 effects that are

    estimable from a 32-run experiment.

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    4/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 584

    Fractionating a Design

    A question that needs to be posed is this, is it very likely that the

    interaction effect between all five factors is significant, especially in

    comparison to the main effects?For that matter, are the

    interactions between three and four factors likely to be thatimportant, or even likely to arise at all given the physical aspects of

    the process or product under investigation?

    If the answers to these questions is no,then we are assuming that

    we are really only interested in estimating the main effects, and

    perhaps the second-order interaction effects between pairs offactors. In this example then, we are executing 32 experimental run

    conditions to estimate only 15 effects at most, plus the constant. Is

    this the most efficient design?

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    5/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 585

    Fractionating a Design

    If we are willing to sacrifice information by mixing presumed

    unimportant effects with important ones, then we can gain efficiency

    by reducing the number of runs necessary to estimate the same

    number of effects.In our example, we have a total of 16 model parameters to estimate

    (potentially, as we are not compelled to include all or any interaction

    terms). Can we select an appropriate subset or fraction of the

    original 32 runs that will still give us this information with the best

    mixture of unimportant and important information?The answer is yesbut which 16 runs do we choose from the

    original 32? The solution is held in the defining relation for fractional

    factorial designs.

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    6/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 586

    Fractionating a Design Example

    Let us look at a simpler experiment that demonstrates how mixtures

    of pieces of information arise in fractional factorial designs.

    Suppose there are four operating variables of interest in a screening

    study and restricted resources permit only eight tests to be carriedout. A full 24design requires 16 runs and it is decided to select the

    eight runs for our experiment from this design.

    There are 12870 different subsets of eight tests that can be selected

    from sixteen, each subset yielding different mixtures of information.

    Which subset should we then select?The particular subset chosen for this demonstration is that for which

    the four factor interaction termx1x2x3x4has the value 1.

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    7/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 587

    Fractionating a Design Example

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    8/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 588

    Fractionating a Design Example

    The eight selected runs can be re-ordered into the more familiar

    format:

    x x x x1 2 3 4

    ! ! ! !

    ! !

    ! !

    ! !

    ! !

    ! !

    ! !

    1 1 1 1

    1 1 1 1

    1 1 1 1

    1 1 1 1

    1 1 1 1

    1 1 1 1

    1 1 1 1

    1 1 1 1

    The pattern of -1s and +1s follows the same full

    factorial pattern as a 23two-level full factorialdesign for the first three factors,x1,x2andx3.

    What about the settings forx4? Is there an easier

    way to determine its values other than writing outthe corresponding 24full factorial design and

    selecting an appropriate number of runs that meetsome criteria such asx1x2x3x4= 1?

    The pattern can be generated from the defining

    relation of the fractional factorial experiment.

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    9/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 589

    The Defining Relation

    The defining relation for a fractional factorial design can be used for

    two purposes:

    Generating the pattern of -1s and +1s for additional factors includedin the experiment beyond the k factors in a 2kfull factorial design.

    Generating the confounding patternof the design. This will be explained

    in more detail later.

    The defining relation for the previous example is I =x1x2x3x4where Iis used to denote a column of 1s.

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    10/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 590

    The Defining Relation Operating Rules

    1. Multiplication of any factor by I leaves the column of values for

    that factor unchanged. For example,

    x1 I =x1 x2x3I =x2x3

    2.

    Multiplication of any factor by itself produces a column of 1s or I.For example,

    x3 x3= (x3)2= I (x1x3x4)

    2= I

    3. Any operation, such as multiplication, that is performed on one

    side of the defining relation equality, must be performed on the

    other side.4. There can be more than one defining relation.

    5. Multiplication of two or more defining relations results in anotherdefining relation.

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    11/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 591

    The Defining Relation Example

    We can use the defining relation from our example, I =x1x2x3x4, to

    generate thex4column.

    So, columnx4is the result of the product of columnsx1,x2andx3.

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    12/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 592

    Confounded Effects

    From the previous example with the defining relation we were left

    withx4=x1x2x3.

    This means that thex4column is identical to that for thex1x2x3

    column.Thus, we cannot independently estimate the effects of bothx4and

    x1x2x3with this experimental design.

    If we include columns in the calculation matrix for both of these

    terms, the resulting matrix will be singular.

    Singular matrices are a problem when software is used for DOEanalysis. The software algorithm will blow up.

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    13/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 593

    Confounded EffectsSince we cannot independently estimate these two effects, we saythat they are confoundedor aliasedwith each other. Theimplications of this confounding is that if, in the analysis of theexperimental results, it is determined that thex4/x1x2x3column has

    a significant effect, we cannot be sure whether the effect is duesolely tox4orx1x2x3or a mixture of the two.

    This is the loss of information (the price you have to pay) forfractionating a design. A smaller number of runs is required, but alleffects cannot be independently estimated in the final analysis.

    However, if third and higher order interaction effects which are notlikely to be important are confounded with main effects and second-order interactions, we can then assume that any effect observed isdue mostly to the main or second-order effect. If we assume that thethird-order interactionx1x2x3is zero, then we can attribute anysignificance solely tox4.

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    14/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 594

    Confounding Pattern

    The fact thatx4is confounded withx1x2x3in our example is not the

    only instance of confounding. When a design is fractionated, all

    effects which can be estimated from the corresponding full factorial

    design are mixed up with each other and the pattern of confoundingcan be derived from the defining relation. For example, if I =

    x1x2x3x4, we have the following confounding pattern:

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    15/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 595

    Confounding Pattern

    Thus each main effect is confounded with a third-order interaction

    (which is probably okay) and the second-order interactions are

    confounded with each other (which may or may not be okay).

    The degree to which main effects and second-order interactions areconfounded with each other or with higher order interactions is

    defined by the resolutionof the design which will be presented

    shortly.

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    16/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 596

    Confounding and the Model

    In the above example, the linear model that can be fit to response

    data can be written as follows:

    Each parameter in the model, !i, i= 1, 2,!, 8 is really estimating

    the mixed effects:

    411431132112443322110

    ~~~~~~~~xxxxxxxxxxY

    !!!!!!!! +++++++=

    ~

    ~

    ~

    ~

    ! ! !

    ! ! !

    ! ! !

    ! ! !

    1 1 234

    2 2 134

    3 3 124

    4 4 123

    = +

    = +

    = +

    = +

    ~

    ~

    ~

    ~

    ! ! !

    ! ! !

    ! ! !

    ! ! !

    0 0 1234

    12 12 34

    13 13 24

    14 14 23

    = +

    = +

    = +

    = +

    ~

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    17/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 597

    Nomenclature

    The number of runs required in two-level full factorial designs is a

    power of two.

    The degree to which a full factorial design is reduced by

    fractionating it is also a power of two. For example, half fractions,quarter fractions, eighth fractions and so on are taken off of full

    factorial designs.

    The order of reduction can be denoted by qwhere 1/2qrepresents

    the degree of fractionation.

    For example, 24-1denotes a half fraction of a 24full factorial design.Algebraically, 24-1= 23= 8 experimental runs, but a 24-1fractionalfactorial design is not the sameas a 23full factorial design which

    also has 8 experimental runs.

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    18/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 598

    Degree of FractionationA full factorial design can only be fractionated to the extent that

    For example, a fractional factorial design involving sevenexperimental factors can be carried out in eight runs as a sixteenthfraction of a 27full factorial design since 27-4= 23= 8 !7 + 1.

    However, the effects of seven experimental factors cannot beestimated in four experimental runs as a 1/32 fraction since27-5= 22= 4 < 7 + 1.

    There are simply not enough degrees of freedom to estimate somany effects with so few runs.

    2 1k q k! " +

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    19/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 599

    Design Resolution

    The resolution of a fractional factorial design describes the extent to

    which main effects and second-order interactions are confounded

    with each other and with third and higher order interactions.

    As the term "resolution" suggests, the higher the resolution thebetter in the sense that potentially important effects are not

    confounded with each other.

    The important effects can be resolved or estimated without worrying

    about the effects they are confounded with.

    Three common classes of resolution for 2k-qfractional factorialdesigns are defined.

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    20/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 600

    Design Resolution

    Resolution III designs are 2k-qdesigns for which

    (i) no individual operating variable, such asx1, is confounded with

    any other individual operating variable, such asx2and

    (ii) at least one individual operating variable is confounded with atwo variable interaction.

    An example of a resolution III design is the 23-1design with definingrelation I =x1x2x3. This is often denoted as:

    23 1

    III

    !

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    21/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 601

    Design Resolution

    Resolution IV designs are 2k-qdesigns for which

    (i) no individual operating variable is confounded with any other

    individual operating variable or with any two variable interaction and

    (ii) at least one two variable interaction is confounded with anothertwo variable interaction.

    An example of a resolution IV design is the 24-1design with definingrelation I =x1x2x3x4. This is often denoted as:

    24 1

    IV

    !

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    22/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 602

    Design Resolution

    Resolution V designs are 2k-qdesigns for which

    (i) no individual operating variable is confounded with any other

    individual operating variable or with any two variable interaction and

    (ii) no two variable interaction is confounded with another twovariable interaction and

    (iii) at least one two variable interaction is confounded with a three

    variable interaction.

    An example is the 25-1design with defining relation I =x1x2x3x4x5.

    This is often denoted as:

    25 1

    V

    !

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    23/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 603

    Catalog of Defining Relations

    Most fractional factorial experiments are 4, 8 or 16 run designs.

    Work has been done by people like George Box to identify the best

    confounding patterns for a variety of fractional factorial designs that

    achieve the highest resolution.The following series of slides provide the defining relations that can

    be used to generate 4, 8 and 16 run fractional factorial designs.

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    24/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 604

    Catalog of Defining Relations

    4 Run Fractional Factorial Designs

    Design Defining Relations Generators132 !

    III 321 xxxI = 213 xxx =

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    25/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 605

    Catalog of Defining Relations

    8 Run Fractional Factorial Designs

    Design Defining Relations Generators14

    2 !IV

    4321 xxxxI = 3214 xxxx =

    252 !III

    531

    421

    xxx

    xxxI

    =

    =

    315

    214

    xxx

    xxx

    =

    =

    362 !III

    632

    531

    421

    xxx

    xxx

    xxxI

    =

    =

    =

    326

    315

    214

    xxx

    xxx

    xxx

    =

    =

    =

    472 !III

    7321

    632

    531

    421

    xxxx

    xxx

    xxx

    xxxI

    =

    =

    =

    =

    3217

    326

    315

    214

    xxxx

    xxx

    xxx

    xxx

    =

    =

    =

    =

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    26/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 606

    Catalog of Defining Relations

    16 Run Fractional Factorial Designs

    Design Defining Relations Generators15

    2 !V

    54321 xxxxxI = 43215 xxxxx =

    262 !IV

    6432

    5321

    xxxx

    xxxxI

    =

    =

    4326

    3215

    xxxx

    xxxx

    =

    =

    372 !IV

    7431

    6432

    5321

    xxxx

    xxxx

    xxxxI

    =

    =

    =

    4317

    4326

    3215

    xxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    =

    =

    =

    482 !

    IV

    8421

    7431

    6432

    5321

    xxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    xxxxI

    =

    =

    =

    =

    4218

    4317

    4326

    3215

    xxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    =

    =

    =

    =

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    27/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 607

    Catalog of Defining Relations

    16 Run Fractional Factorial Designs

    Design Defining Relations Generators592 !

    III

    94321

    8421

    7431

    6432

    5321

    xxxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    xxxxI

    =

    =

    =

    =

    =

    43219

    4218

    4317

    4326

    3215

    xxxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    =

    =

    =

    =

    =

    6102 !III

    94321

    8421

    7431

    6432

    5321

    xxxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    xxxxI

    =

    =

    =

    =

    =

    1021 xxxI =

    43219

    4218

    4317

    4326

    3215

    xxxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    =

    =

    =

    =

    =

    2110 xxx =

    9 52

    IV

    !

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    28/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 608

    Catalog of Defining Relations

    16 Run Fractional Factorial Designs

    Design Defining Relations Generators7112 !

    III

    94321

    8421

    7431

    6432

    5321

    xxxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    xxxxI

    =

    =

    =

    =

    =

    1131

    1021

    xxx

    xxxI

    =

    =

    43219

    4218

    4317

    4326

    3215

    xxxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    =

    =

    =

    =

    =

    3111

    2110

    xxx

    xxx

    =

    =

    8122 !III

    94321

    8421

    7431

    6432

    5321

    xxxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    xxxxI

    =

    =

    =

    =

    =

    1241

    1131

    1021

    xxx

    xxx

    xxxI

    =

    =

    =

    43219

    4218

    4317

    4326

    3215

    xxxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    =

    =

    =

    =

    =

    4112

    3111

    2110

    xxx

    xxx

    xxx

    =

    =

    =

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    29/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 609

    Catalog of Defining Relations

    16 Run Fractional Factorial Designs

    Design Defining Relations Generators9132 !

    III

    94321

    8421

    7431

    6432

    5321

    xxxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    xxxxI

    =

    =

    =

    =

    =

    1332

    1241

    1131

    1021

    xxx

    xxx

    xxx

    xxxI

    =

    =

    =

    =

    43219

    4218

    4317

    4326

    3215

    xxxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    =

    =

    =

    =

    =

    3213

    4112

    3111

    2110

    xxx

    xxx

    xxx

    xxx

    =

    =

    =

    =

    10142 !III

    94321

    8421

    7431

    6432

    5321

    xxxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    xxxxI

    =

    =

    =

    =

    =

    1442

    1332

    1241

    1131

    1021

    xxx

    xxx

    xxx

    xxx

    xxxI

    =

    =

    =

    =

    =

    43219

    4218

    4317

    4326

    3215

    xxxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    =

    =

    =

    =

    =

    4214

    3213

    4112

    3111

    2110

    xxx

    xxx

    xxx

    xxx

    xxx

    =

    =

    =

    =

    =

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    30/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 610

    Catalog of Defining Relations

    16 Run Fractional Factorial Designs

    Design Defining Relations Generators11152 !

    III

    94321

    8421

    7431

    6432

    5321

    xxxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    xxxxI

    =

    =

    =

    =

    =

    1543

    1442

    1332

    1241

    1131

    1021

    xxx

    xxx

    xxx

    xxx

    xxx

    xxxI

    =

    =

    =

    =

    =

    =

    43219

    4218

    4317

    4326

    3215

    xxxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    xxxx

    =

    =

    =

    =

    =

    4315

    4214

    3213

    4112

    3111

    2110

    xxx

    xxx

    xxx

    xxx

    xxx

    xxx

    =

    =

    =

    =

    =

    =

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    31/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 611

    Minitab Exercise Fractional Factorial Designs

    Click on Stat!DOE!Factorial!Create Factorial Design

    Click on the Display Available Designs button

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    32/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 612

    Defining Relations and Confounding Patterns

    Rule 5 governing defining relations states that a new defining

    relation can be determined by multiplying defining relations together.

    For example, a resolution III 25-2design has defining relations

    I =x1x2x4=x1x3x5. A third defining relation can be found bymultiplying these two together.

    ( )

    ( )

    5432

    5432

    5432

    2

    1

    531421

    xxxx

    xxxxI

    xxxxx

    xxxxxxI

    =

    =

    =

    = There can be many defining relationsthat determine the complete

    confounding pattern for highlyfractionated designs (e.g. 215-11).

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    33/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 613

    Defining Relations and Confounding PatternsFrom the previous 25-2example where we haveI =x1x2x4=x1x3x5=x2x3x4x5, we have the following confounding (oraliasing) pattern:

    x1withx2x4 andx3x5x2withx1x4x3withx1x5x4withx1x2x5withx1x3x2x3withx4x5

    x2x5withx3x4

    We are usually only interested in the confounding pattern up tosecond-order interactions.

    This confounding pattern accountsfor the relationships between all

    five main effects and the tensecond-order interactions.

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    34/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 614

    Defining Relations and Confounding Patterns

    If we fit the following proposed model for a resolution III 25-2design:

    Each parameter in the model, !i, i= 1, 2,!

    , 8 is really estimatingthe mixed effects:

    5225322355443322110

    ~~~~~~~~xxxxxxxxxY !!!!!!!! +++++++=

    1355

    1244

    1533

    1422

    352411

    ~

    ~

    ~

    ~

    ~

    !!!

    !!!

    !!!

    !!!

    !!!!

    +=

    +=

    +=

    +=

    ++=

    342525

    452323

    234513512400

    ~

    ~

    ~

    !!!

    !!!

    !!!!!

    +=

    +=

    +++=

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    35/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 615

    Interactions

    For resolution V designs, all of the main effects and second-order

    interactions can be estimated independentlyin that they are

    confounded with third and higher-order interactions that are

    assumed to be insignificant.However, it is possible in some situations to obtain independent

    estimates of second-order interactions in lower resolution designs.

    It depends on the extent of prior knowledge the experimenter has with

    the system/process/product/design.

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    36/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 616

    Interactions Example

    Lets use the previous 25-2example where we have

    I =x1x2x4=x1x3x5=x2x3x4x5. Ignoring second-order interactions, the

    main effects model that can be fit is:

    The alias pattern for the main effects is:

    55443322110~~~~~~ xxxxxY !!!!!! +++++=

    1355

    1244

    1533

    1422

    352411

    ~

    ~

    ~

    ~

    ~

    !!!

    !!!

    !!!

    !!!

    !!!!

    +=

    +=

    +=

    +=

    ++=

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    37/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 617

    Interactions Example

    Note that we still have the freedom to include two more terms in the

    proposed model corresponding to the confounded pairs of second-

    order interactions (!23, !45) and (!25, !34).

    Suppose that the five factors in the experiment are nitrogen contentin lawn fertilizer, amount of lawn watering, lawn aeration, type of

    grass in the lawn and grade of top soil.

    The response of interest is the total weight of grass clippings after a

    season of mowing. More grass clippings is suggestive of a healthier,

    thicker lawn.As an experienced gardener, you strongly suspect there to be a

    significant interaction between nitrogen content and amount of

    watering.

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    38/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 618

    Interactions Example

    The design initially proposed to you, based on the defining relations,

    is as follows:

    11111

    11111

    11111

    11111

    11111

    11111

    11111

    11111

    315214321

    !!!

    !!

    !!!

    !!

    !!!

    !!!!

    !!!

    == xxxxxxxxx

    x1= nitrogen content

    x2= aeration

    x3= grade of top soil

    x4= amount of water

    x5= type of grass

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    39/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 619

    Interactions Example

    You see right away that the second-order interaction of interest,

    between nitrogen content and amount of watering, corresponds to

    thex1x4interaction the way the design is presently defined.

    From examining the confounding pattern, you notice that thex1x4interaction is confounded with the main effect forx2.

    Hence, as currently defined, the proposed design will not meet your

    modeling objective.

    However, by judicially reassigning the experimental factors, you can

    make use of one of the two pairs of second-order interaction pairsthat are not in the model yet.

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    40/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 620

    Interactions Example

    You propose the following changes:

    11111

    11111

    11111

    11111

    11111

    11111

    11111

    11111

    315214321

    !!!

    !!

    !!!

    !!

    !!!

    !!!!

    !!!

    == xxxxxxxxx

    x1= aerationx2= nitrogen content

    x3= amount of water

    x4= grade of top soil

    x5= type of grass

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    41/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 621

    Interactions Example

    Now the interaction of interest corresponds tox2x3which can be

    added to the model without having it confounded with any of the five

    main effects:

    Depending on your prior knowledge about the process under

    investigation, it may still be possible to obtain "independent"

    estimates of main effects and some second-order interactions, even

    with low resolution designs, if care is taken in the design stage toobtain a desirable confounding pattern.

    The experimental design and subsequent confounding relationshipscan always be evaluated before committing to run the experiment.

    322355443322110~~~~~~~ xxxxxxxY !!!!!!! ++++++=

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    42/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 622

    Saturated Designs

    In the previous example, it was possible to use extra degrees of

    freedom to include one or more interaction terms in the model.

    There are a group of resolution III designs known as saturated

    designsthat do not afford this freedom.With a design from this group, koperating variables can be

    investigated simultaneously in k+1 tests where k+1 is a power of 2.

    Examples of saturated two-level fractional factorial designs are:

    In these designs, 3, 7, 15 and 31 factors are investigated in 4, 8, 16

    and 32 runs respectively.

    Experimental Designs for Screening

    3 1 7 4 15 11 31 262 , 2 , 2 , 2

    III III III III

    ! ! ! !

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    43/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 623

    Saturated Designs Example

    Construction of a resolution III 27-4design is accomplished by first

    writing a 23design in three of the seven operating variables,X1,X2

    andX3.

    Each of the remaining operating variables,X4,X5,X6andX7isconfounded with an interaction amongX1,X2andX3.

    If the aliasesX4=X1X2,X5=X1X3,X6=X2X3andX7=X1X2X3are

    used, then the resulting design is that shown below.

    Experimental Designs for Screening

    x x x x x x x1 2 3 4 5 6 7

    ! ! ! !

    ! ! ! !

    ! ! ! !

    ! ! ! !

    ! ! ! !

    ! ! ! !

    ! ! ! !

    1 1 1 1 1 1 1

    1 1 1 1 1 1 1

    1 1 1 1 1 1 1

    1 1 1 1 1 1 1

    1 1 1 1 1 1 1

    1 1 1 1 1 1 1

    1 1 1 1 1 1 1

    1 1 1 1 1 1 1

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    44/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 624

    Saturated Designs Example

    From the four basic generatorsX1X2X4,X1X3X5,X2X3X6and

    X1X2X3X7arising from the choice of aliases, the following defining

    relation for this design can be formed,

    Experimental Designs for Screening

    I x x x x x x x x x x x x x

    x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x

    x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x

    = = = =

    = = = = = =

    = = = =

    1 2 4 1 3 5 2 3 6 1 2 3 7

    2 3 4 5 1 3 4 6 3 4 7 1 2 5 6 2 5 7 1 6 7

    4 5 6 1 4 5 7 2 4 6 7 3 5 6 7

    (taking basic generators

    one at a time)

    (products of two basic

    generators)

    (products of three basic

    generators)

    (products of four basic

    generators)

    = x x x x x x x1 2 3 4 5 6 7

    Note: These are also referred to as the words of a defining relation.

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    45/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 625

    Saturated Designs Example

    Notice that the smallest order interaction in this defining relation is

    three, verifying that the resolution of the design is indeed III.

    Again ignoring interactions involving more than two operating

    variables, the following eight estimates can be obtained from thisdesign.

    Experimental Designs for Screening

    ( )( )( )

    ( )( )( )( )

    l

    l

    l

    l

    l

    l

    l

    l

    1 24 35 67

    2 14 36 57

    3 15 26 47

    4 12 56 37

    5 13 46 27

    6 23 45 17

    7 34 25 16

    0 0

    ,

    ,

    ,

    ,

    ,

    ,

    ,

    ,

    which estimates

    which estimates

    which estimates

    which estimates

    which estimates

    which estimates

    which estimates

    which estimates

    1

    2

    3

    4

    5

    6

    7

    ! ! ! !

    ! ! ! !

    ! ! ! !

    ! ! ! !! ! ! !

    ! ! ! !

    ! ! ! !

    !

    + + +

    + + +

    + + +

    + + +

    + + +

    + + +

    + + +

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    46/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 626

    Saturated Designs Interpretation of Results

    Interpretation of results from a saturated design may be ambiguous

    because each operating variable is confounded with a number of

    two variable interactions.

    As will be shown later, ambiguities can be partially resolved bycarrying out another saturated design from the same "family", that is,

    a design for which the signs of all values of one or more of the

    operating variables are reversed.

    Thus, saturated designs are more useful for the first step in a

    screening study of several operating variables.

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    47/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 627

    Example Saturated Design

    This example has been slightly modified from the Course Notes:

    Referring to Table 23.5 in the notes, the following changes have been

    made:

    x4

    = recycle

    x5= rate of addition of NaOH

    x6= type of filter cloth

    x7= holdup time

    The standard generators ofx4=x1x2,x5=x1x3,x6=x2x3andx7=x1x2x3

    are used instead of the ones in Equation 23.3.

    The columns in the design matrix in Table 23.6 have been accordinglyadjusted.

    "s are used in stead of !s for the model coefficients.

    None of these changes affects the resulting analysis and interpretation.

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    48/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 628

    Example Saturated Design

    During the startup of a new process in a chemical plant, trouble was

    encountered in a filtration operation. Filtration was requiring about

    70 minutes per batch instead of 40 minutes, the required time for a

    similar operation at other plant sites. An investigation was

    undertaken to identify the operating variables that affected filtration

    time and to determine how these variables might be altered in order

    to reduce the filtration time. The operating variables selected for the

    initial study are shown in the following table. The low levels

    represent the operating conditions prior to this screening study. The

    high levels are changes in operating conditions chosen to identifywhich, if any, of these seven operating variables affected the

    filtration time. It will be noted that four of the operating variables,x1,

    x2,x4andx6are qualitative variables.

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    49/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 629

    Example Saturated Design

    Model to be fit:

    Operating Variable Level-1 1

    x1 , water supply municipal reservoir well

    x2 , raw material made on site made at another site

    x3 , filtration temperature low high

    x4 , recycle included omitted

    x5 , rate of addition of NaOH fast slow

    x6 , type of filter cloth new old

    x7 , holdup time short long

    ( ) 776655443322110 xxxxxxxYE !!!!!!!! +++++++=

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    50/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 630

    Example Saturated Design

    As a first step in the study, a 27-4resolution III design was employed

    because of its economy in tests and its facility for use as a building

    block for further tests that might be required.

    Basic generators chosen for the design were:

    3217

    326

    315

    214

    7321632531421

    xxxx

    xxx

    xxx

    xxx

    xxxxxxxxxxxxxI

    =

    =

    =

    =

    ====

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    51/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 631

    Example Saturated Design

    The design and the measured steady state filtration time for each

    test are:

    (min.)7654321 yxxxxxxx

    7.38

    7.68

    2.41

    6.78

    0.81

    4.66

    7.77

    4.68

    1111111

    1111111

    1111111

    1111111

    1111111

    1111111

    1111111

    1111111

    !!!!

    !!!!

    !!!!

    !!!!

    !!!!

    !!!!

    !!!!

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    52/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 632

    Example Saturated Design

    When these results were examined, there may well have been a

    temptation to conclude that either the sixth test or the eighth test

    from the experiment had resolved the problem since both tests

    produced filtration times in the order of 40 minutes, the target figure.

    As will be shown shortly, a conclusion that changes in x1, x3and x5

    produced this favourable result is only one of several possible

    interpretations of these data.

    In any case, before making a change in such an important operating

    variable as water supply (x1), other interpretations would have to beassessed.

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    53/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 633

    Example Minitab Output

    Fitted coefficients are:

    The fitted model is then:

    Estimated Effects and Coefficients for Filt. (coded units)

    Term Effect Coef

    Constant 65.09

    Water Su -10.87 -5.44

    Raw Mate -2.77 -1.39

    Filt. Te -16.58 -8.29

    Recycle 3.17 1.59

    NaOH Add -22.83 -11.41

    Type Fil -3.42 -1.71

    Holdup 0.53 0.26

    7654321 26.071.141.1159.129.839.144.509.65 xxxxxxxy +!!+!!!=

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    54/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 634

    Example Saturated Design

    From the defining relation for this design it can be confirmed that the

    eight coefficient estimates are confounded with a number of two-

    factor interactions. Interactions involving more than two operating

    variables have been ignored.

    x1+ x

    2x4+ x

    3x5+ x

    6x7

    x2+ x

    1x4+ x

    3x6+ x

    5x7

    x3+ x

    1x5+ x

    2x6+ x

    4x7

    x4+ x

    1x2+ x

    3x7+ x

    5x6

    x5+ x1x3+ x2x7+ x4x6x6+ x

    1x7+ x

    2x3+ x

    4x5

    x7+ x

    1x6+ x

    2x5+ x

    3x4

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    55/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 635

    Example Saturated Design

    Because further tests were carried out in this study, a second

    subscript has been added to these estimates to denote that they

    arise from the first set of tests.

    ( )( )( )

    ( )

    ( )

    ( )( )( ) 26.0,

    71.1,

    41.11,

    59.1,

    29.8,

    39.1,

    44.5,

    09.65,

    342516771

    452317661

    462713551

    563712441

    472615331

    573614221

    673524111

    001

    =+++

    !=+++

    !=+++

    =+++

    !=+++

    !=+++

    !=+++

    =

    """"#

    """"#

    """"#

    """"#

    """"#

    """"#

    """"#

    "#

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    56/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 636

    Example Saturated Design

    Because no estimate of the pure error variance is available, one

    interpretation of these estimates can be made on the basis of their

    relative magnitudes. Among the coefficients of operating variables,

    the estimates "5

    , "3

    and "1

    are much larger in magnitude than the

    other estimates. A number of alternative interpretations are possible.

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    57/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 637

    Example Saturated Design

    A simple explanation of these three large estimates might be that

    only the terms !1x1, !3x3and !5x5are important, all two variable

    interactions being of negligible size.

    Another explanation might be that only operating variablesx1andx3are affecting the filtration time, their influence being explained by

    terms !1x1, !3x3and !13x1x3.

    A third possibility is that only operating variablesx1andx5are

    important, their effect being accounted for by terms !1x1, !5x5and

    !15x1x5.Another alternative is that only operating variablesx3andx5are

    causing the response to change via terms !3x3, !5x5and !35x3x5.

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    58/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 638

    Example Saturated Design

    ( )( )

    ( )

    ( )( )

    ( )( )

    ( ) 26.0,71.1,

    41.11,

    59.1,

    29.8,

    39.1,

    44.5,

    09.65,

    342516771

    452317661

    462713551

    563712441

    472615331

    573614221

    673524111

    001

    =+++

    !=+++

    !=+++

    =+++

    !=+++

    !=+++

    !=+++

    =

    """"#

    """"#

    """"#

    """"#

    """"#

    """"#

    """"#

    "#

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean ( )( )

    ( )( )

    ( )

    ( )( )

    ( ) 26.0,71.1,

    41.11,

    59.1,

    29.8,

    39.1,

    44.5,

    09.65,

    342516771

    452317661

    462713551

    563712441

    472615331

    573614221

    673524111

    001

    =+++

    !=+++

    !=+++

    =+++

    !=+++

    !=+++

    !=+++

    =

    """"#

    """"#

    """"#

    """"#

    """"#

    """"#

    """"#

    "#

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ( )

    ( )

    ( )( )( )

    ( )( )

    ( ) 26.0,71.1,

    41.11,

    59.1,

    29.8,39.1,

    44.5,

    09.65,

    342516771

    452317661

    462713551

    563712441

    472615331

    573614221

    673524111

    001

    =+++

    !=+++

    !=+++

    =+++

    !=+++!

    =+++

    !=+++

    =

    """"#

    """"#

    """"#

    """"#

    """"#""""#

    """"#

    "#

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimateanofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean ( )

    ( )

    ( )( )( )

    ( )

    ( )( ) 26.0,

    71.1,

    41.11,

    59.1,

    29.8,39.1,

    44.5,

    09.65,

    342516771

    452317661

    462713551

    563712441

    472615331

    573614221

    673524111

    001

    =+++

    !=+++

    !=+++

    =+++

    !=+++

    !

    =+++

    !=+++

    =

    """"#

    """"#

    """"#

    """"#

    """"#""""#

    """"#

    "#

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimateanofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    59/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 639

    Minitab Exercise Saturated Experiment

    Open the file Topic06SatExp.MTW

    Select Stat!DOE!Factorial!Analyze Factorial Design

    Select C12 Y as the Response

    Click on the Graphs buttonClick on the Four in one radio button

    Click on OK

    Click on OK

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    60/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 640

    Minitab output

    Experimental Designs for Screening

    Analysis of Variance

    Source DF Adj SS Adj MS F-Value P-Value

    Model 7 1887.53 269.65 * *

    Linear 7 1887.53 269.65 * *

    X1 1 236.53 236.53 * *

    X2 1 15.40 15.40 * *X3 1 549.46 549.46 * *

    X4 1 20.16 20.16 * *

    X5 1 1041.96 1041.96 * *

    X6 1 23.46 23.46 * *

    X7 1 0.55 0.55 * *

    Error 0 * *

    Total 7 1887.53

    Model Summary

    S R-sq R-sq(adj) R-sq(pred)

    * 100.00% * *

    Note that test statistics

    and p-values cannot be

    calculated because this

    is an exact fit as

    exhibited by an R2

    of100%.

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    61/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 641

    Minitab output

    Experimental Designs for Screening

    Coded Coefficients

    SE

    Term Effect Coef Coef T-Value P-Value VIF

    Constant 65.09 * * *

    X1 -10.875 -5.437 * * * 1.00

    X2 -2.775 -1.387 * * * 1.00

    X3 -16.575 -8.288 * * * 1.00

    X4 3.175 1.587 * * * 1.00

    X5 -22.82 -11.41 * * * 1.00

    X6 -3.425 -1.712 * * * 1.00

    X7 0.5250 0.2625 * * * 1.00

    Regression Equation in Uncoded Units

    Y = 65.09 - 5.437 X1 - 1.387 X2 - 8.288 X3 + 1.587 X4 - 11.41 X5 - 1.712 X6

    + 0.2625 X7

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    62/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 642

    Minitab output

    Experimental Designs for Screening

    Aliases

    I + ABD + ACE + AFG + BCF + BEG + CDG + DEF

    A + BD + CE + FG + BCG + BEF + CDF + DEG

    B + AD + CF + EG + ACG + AEF + CDE + DFG

    C + AE + BF + DG + ABG + ADF + BDE + EFG

    D + AB + CG + EF + ACF + AEG + BCE + BFG

    E + AC + BG + DF + ABF + ADG + BCD + CFG

    F + AG + BC + DE + ABE + ACD + BDG + CEG

    G + AF + BE + CD + ABC + ADE + BDF + CEF

    * NOTE * Could not graph the specified residual type because MSE = 0 or the

    degrees of freedom for error = 0.

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    63/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 643

    Example Foldover DesignIt is well known that low resolution fractional factorial designs (III andIV) confound main effects with second-order interactions (III) andsecond-order interactions with each other (IV).

    This sometimes makes the interpretation of a DOE analysis difficult.

    Foldover designs can be used to increase the resolution of a lowresolution design and help to resolve lingering questions from theinitial design.

    Foldover designs are a nice sequential strategy to employ whenresources are limited.

    Because of the ambiguity in interpreting the results of these tests, asecond foldoverset of eight tests was conducted using a 27-4resolution III design formed from the first design by reversing thesigns of all values for all seven operating variables.

    This second design is shown on the next slide along with themeasured filtration times obtained for these additional tests.

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    64/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 644

    Example Foldover Design

    (min.)7654321 yxxxxxxx

    6.67

    6.42

    0.59

    8.47

    9.61

    4.860.65

    7.66

    1111111

    1111111

    1111111

    1111111

    1111111

    11111111111111

    1111111

    !!!!!!!

    !!!

    !!!

    !!!

    !!!

    !!!

    !!!

    !!!

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    65/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 645

    Example Foldover Design

    Switching the signs of all values for one operating variablexiin a

    27-4resolution III design is equivalent to replacingxiwith -xi.

    Because of the manner in which this second design has been

    constructed from the first design, its defining relation can be

    obtained by replacing every operating variablexiin the defining

    relation for the first design, by -xi. The resulting defining relation is

    then:

    3217

    326

    315

    214

    7321632531421

    xxxx

    xxx

    xxx

    xxx

    xxxxxxxxxxxxxI

    =

    !=

    !=

    !

    =

    =!=!=!=

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    66/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 646

    Example Minitab OutputMinitab was used to obtain the following results for the second

    design on its own:

    Estimated Effects and Coefficients for Filt. (coded units)

    Term Effect Coef

    Constant 62.125

    Water Su -2.500 -1.250

    Raw Mate -5.000 -2.500

    Filt. Te 15.750 7.875

    Recycle 2.250 1.125

    NaOH Add -15.600 -7.800

    Type Fil 3.300 1.650Holdup -9.150 -4.575

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    67/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 647

    Example Foldover DesignFrom the defining relation for the foldover design it can be confirmed

    that the eight coefficient estimates are confounded in the following

    matter. Interactions involving more than two operating variables

    have been ignored.

    x1- x

    2x4- x

    3x5- x

    6x7

    x2- x

    1x4- x

    3x6- x

    5x7

    x3- x

    1x5- x

    2x6- x

    4x7

    x4- x

    1x2- x

    3x7- x

    5x6

    x5- x1x3- x2x7- x4x6x6- x

    1x7- x

    2x3- x

    4x5

    x7- x

    1x6- x

    2x5- x

    3x4

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    68/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 648

    Example Foldover DesignSo, we have:

    which creates even more possibilities.

    ( )( )( )( )( )( )

    ( )( ) 575.4,65.1,

    80.7,

    125.1,

    875.7,

    50.2,

    25.1,

    125.62,

    342516772

    452317662

    462713552

    563712442

    472615332

    573614222

    673524112

    002

    !=!!!

    =!!!

    !=!!!

    =!!!

    =!!!

    !=!!!

    !=!!!

    =

    """"#""""#

    """"#

    """"#

    """"#

    """"#

    """"#

    "#

    ofestimateanofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    69/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 649

    Example Foldover DesignHowever, we can combine the results from the original design with

    those from the foldover design in the following manner:

    ( )( )( )

    ( )

    ( )( )

    ( ) 16.2,203.0,261.9,2

    36.1,2

    21.0,2

    94.1,2

    34.3,2

    6.63,2

    77271

    66261

    55251

    44241

    33231

    22221

    11211

    00201

    !=+

    !=+

    !

    =+

    =+

    !=+

    !=+

    !=+

    =+

    "##

    "##"##

    "##

    "##

    "##

    "##

    "##

    ofestimatean

    ofestimateanofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    70/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 650

    Example Foldover Design

    ( ) 5.1,,20201

    =! effectblocktheofestimatean""

    ( ) ( )( ) ( )( ) ( )

    ( ) ( )( ) ( )( ) ( )( ) ( ) 09.2,2

    68.1,2

    42.2,2

    08.8,2

    56.0,2

    81.1,2

    231.0,2

    6735241211

    4523176261

    3425167271

    4726153231

    5736142221

    4627135251

    5637124241

    !=++!

    !=++!

    =++!

    !=++

    !

    =++!

    !=++!

    =++!

    """##

    """##

    """##

    """##

    """##

    """##

    """##

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    ofestimatean

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    71/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 651

    Example Foldover DesignThe block effectis the difference in average response values

    between the two sets of eight runs.

    Had it been large, it would have indicated the presence of some

    other variables, beyond the seven being studied, whose change

    between the two sets of tests strongly affected the filtration time.

    By combining the original with the foldover experiment, we have

    effectively created a 27-3resolution IV experimental design.

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    72/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 652

    Example Foldover DesignAmong the above sixteen estimates, the largest is -9.61, an estimateof !5and 8.08, an estimate of the linear combination of two variableinteractions !15+ !26+ !47. The next largest estimate is -3.34, anestimate of !1.

    The investigators concluded that operating variables !1and !5alone,the water supply and the rate of addition of caustic soda, affected thefiltration time, and the estimate -8.08 occurred primarily because of theinteraction !1!5.

    Even at this stage, of course, other interpretations are possible. Forexample, the estimate -8.08 might have been due to the interactionx2x6and/or the interactionx4x7. It is noted, however, that the estimatesof !2, !6, !4, and !7are all relatively small and, although this does notnecessarily mean that interactions among these variables must alsobe small, this is often the case.

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    73/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 653

    Example Foldover DesignThe investigatorsinterpretation can be summarized conveniently

    by the following chart which shows the average filtration time

    obtained at each of the four sets of operating conditions of water

    supply and rate of addition of NaOH. Evidence of the large negative

    interaction between the two variables is very strong.

    reservoir well

    slow

    fast

    water supply

    rate of addition

    of NaOH

    68.5 78.0

    65.4 42.6

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    74/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 654

    Example Foldover DesignThe corrective action implied by these results was to change the

    water supply from the municipal reservoir to the well and reduce the

    rate of addition of caustic soda. These changes were made and

    satisfactory filtration times close to 40 minutes were obtained in

    subsequent plant operation.

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    75/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 655

    Exercise Foldover DesignThe reduced fitted model from this example is:

    What is the predicted filtration time at the new operating conditionsusing well water and a slow rate of NaOH addition?

    5151 08.861.934.36.63 xxxxy !!!=

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    76/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 656

    Minitab Exercise Foldover ExperimentClick on Stat!DOE!Factorial!Create Factorial Design

    Use the Number of factors drop down menu to select 7

    Click on the Designs button

    Select the first row for a 2^7-4 fractional factorial design

    Click OK

    Click on the Factors button

    Change the Factor Names from A through G to X1 through X7

    Click OK

    Click on the Options button

    Click on the Fold on all factors radio button

    Uncheck the Randomize runs box

    Click OK

    Click OK

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    77/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 657

    Minitab Exercise Foldover ExperimentEnter the Y response data in the Minitab worksheet

    Experimental Designs for Screening

    X1 X2 X3 X4 X5 X6 X7 Y

    -1 -1 -1 1 1 1 -1 68.4

    1 -1 -1 -1 -1 1 1 77.7

    -1 1 -1 -1 1 -1 1 66.4

    1 1 -1 1 -1 -1 -1 81.0

    -1 -1 1 1 -1 -1 1 78.6

    1 -1 1 -1 1 -1 -1 41.2

    -1 1 1 -1 -1 1 -1 68.7

    1 1 1 1 1 1 1 38.7

    1 1 1 -1 -1 -1 1 66.7

    -1 1 1 1 1 -1 -1 65.01 -1 1 1 -1 1 -1 86.4

    -1 -1 1 -1 1 1 1 61.9

    1 1 -1 -1 1 1 -1 47.8

    -1 1 -1 1 -1 1 1 59.0

    1 -1 -1 1 1 -1 1 42.6

    -1 -1 -1 -1 -1 -1 -1 67.6

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    78/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 658

    Minitab Exercise Foldover ExperimentSelect Stat!DOE!Factorial!Analyze Factorial Design

    Select C12 Y as the Response

    Click on the Graphs button

    Click on the Four in one radio button

    Click on OK

    Click on OK

    Experimental Designs for Screening

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    79/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 659

    Minitab Exercise Foldover ExperimentCoefficients table

    Experimental Designs for Screening

    Coded Coefficients

    SE

    Term Effect Coef Coef T-Value P-Value VIF

    Constant 63.61 * * *X1 -6.687 -3.344 * * * 1.00

    X2 -3.888 -1.944 * * * 1.00

    X3 -0.4125 -0.2062 * * * 1.00

    X4 2.712 1.356 * * * 1.00

    X5 -19.213 -9.606 * * * 1.00

    X6 -0.06250 -0.03125 * * * 1.00

    X7 -4.313 -2.156 * * * 1.00

    X1*X2 0.4625 0.2312 * * * 1.00

    X1*X3 -3.613 -1.806 * * * 1.00

    X1*X4 1.1125 0.5563 * * * 1.00

    X1*X5 -16.163 -8.081 * * * 1.00

    X1*X6 4.838 2.419 * * * 1.00

    X1*X7 -3.362 -1.681 * * * 1.00

    X2*X4 -4.188 -2.094 * * * 1.00

    X1*X2*X4 2.963 1.481 * * * 1.00

    f S

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    80/81

    N.J. Burn & Associates Inc. 660

    Minitab Exercise Foldover ExperimentReduced model

    Start removing higher order terms with the smallest magnitude

    coefficient value (highest p-value)

    Experimental Designs for Screening

    Analysis of Variance

    Source DF Adj SS Adj MS F-Value P-Value

    Model 3 2700.3 900.09 23.13 0.000

    Linear 2 1655.4 827.69 21.27 0.000

    X1 1 178.9 178.89 4.60 0.053

    X5 1 1476.5 1476.48 37.94 0.000

    2-Way Interactions 1 1044.9 1044.91 26.85 0.000

    X1*X5 1 1044.9 1044.91 26.85 0.000

    Error 12 467.1 38.92Total 15 3167.3

    E i l D i f S i

  • 7/26/2019 Handout_10 SV(1)

    81/81

    Minitab Exercise Foldover Experiment

    Experimental Designs for Screening

    Model Summary

    S R-sq R-sq(adj) R-sq(pred)

    6.23867 85.25% 81.57% 73.78%

    Coded Coefficients

    Term Effect Coef SE Coef T-Value P-Value VIF

    Constant 63.61 1.56 40.78 0.000

    X1 -6.69 -3.34 1.56 -2.14 0.053 1.00

    X5 -19.21 -9.61 1.56 -6.16 0.000 1.00

    X1*X5 -16.16 -8.08 1.56 -5.18 0.000 1.00

    Regression Equation in Uncoded Units

    Y = 63.61 - 3.34 X1 - 9.61 X5 - 8.08 X1*X5