Basden Basden 1996

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/3/2019 Basden Basden 1996

    1/23

  • 8/3/2019 Basden Basden 1996

    2/23

    retrieval inhibition as an additional mechanism, but did not tie it to a specific

    method. We have argued (Basden et al., 1994; Basden, B asden, & Gargano,

    1993) that different mechanisms are responsible for directed forgetting with the

    two methods, but we have proposed that retrieval inhibition underlies directed

    forgetting with the list method, and that differential processing of Remember

    and Forget targets underlies directed forgetting with the item method. Inaddition, we have assumed that relational (organisation) processing occurs to a

    greater extent with the list method than with the item method, and that item-

    specific processing occurs to a greater extent with the item method than with the

    list method. In a com prehensive review of the directed forgetting literature,

    Johnson (1994) drew a similar conclusion. She attributed greater recall of

    Remember than of Forget targets with the list method to segregation and

    selective search of each list of targets followed by inhibition of the to-be-

    forgotten list. With the item method, segregation and retrieval inhibition are not

    required because only the set of Remember targets is stored and searched.

    Three lines of evidence support the position that retrieval inhibition occurs

    with the list method and not with the item method. First, the item method

    typically yields greater directed forgetting than the list method on recall tests, a

    result that should be expected if Forget targets are more extensively processed

    with the list method than with the item method. Second, the item method yields

    directed forgetting on recognition tests, but the list method does not. If items are

    less well processed at storage (as we believe is the case with the item method),

    recognition memory should be affected. If a set of equally well-processed items

    is inhibited from retrieval (as we believe in the case of the list method), a test

    that de-emphasises retrieval should be unaffected. Third, the re-presentation of

    targets on a recognition test seems to effect a ``release from inhibition on a

    subsequent recall test when the list method is used, but not when the item

    method is used1

    (e.g. Basden et al., 1993; Bjork, 1989). Re-presentation of both

    Remember and Forget items would not be expected to eliminate a processingdeficit for the Forget items, but might very well overcome retrieval inhibition.

    According to this distinction between relational and item-specific processing

    (Einstein & Hunt, 1980; Hunt & Einstein, 1981), procedures that focus the

    subjects attention on the list as a whole (or at least on large sections of it)

    encourage the identification of relationships among the i tems, whereas

    procedures that focus the subjects attention on individual items emphasise

    information that is item-specific. T hus, the list m ethod of directed forgetting

    w o uld e nc our ag e r ela ti on a l p roc e ssi ng w he re as th e it em m e th od w ou ldencourage item-specific processing. This is not to say, of course, that either

    634 B A S D E N A N D B A S D E N

  • 8/3/2019 Basden Basden 1996

    3/23

    suggesting that the mix of relational and item-specific information is influence

    by the method of directed forgetting. Relational processing should facilitate lis

    method directed forgetting because in this case suppression of an entire, alread

    processed list must occur. Item-specific processing should facilitate ite

    method directed forgetting because in this case individual targets must b

    forgotten at the time they are undergoing processing.If the item method encourages item-specific processing and the list metho

    encourages relational processing, and if directed forgetting results from

    processing deficit with the item method and from retrieval inhibition with th

    list method, then factors that enhance item-specific processing should increa

    the magnitude of directed forgetting with the item method, and factors th

    enhance relational processing should increase directed forgetting with the li

    method. In Experiment 1, we manipulated the study format as a means

    encouraging either relational or item-specific processing. Our expectation w

    that the relative magnitude of directed forgetting with the two methods wou

    vary with study format.

    E X P E R IM E N T 1

    In this experiment one group of subjects attempted to memorise a set of anim

    pictures and two other groups of subjects attempted to memorise a list of nam

    for those pictures. The subjects in one of the two latter groups w ere given simp

    learning instructions and those in the other group were told to form a ment

    image of the referent of each target. A few prior studies have employed imag

    or pictures in the context of directed forgetting (Bray, Justice, & Zahm, 198

    Bugelski, 1970). Unfortunately, no firm conclusions could be drawn from the

    previous studies.

    According to Hunt and his colleagues, item-specific processing is encourage

    either by forming images of the targets (Hunt & Marschark, 1989) or bstudying pictures corresponding to the targets (Hunt & McDaniel, 1993). Hu

    and McDaniel have also argued that simple presentation of a categorised list f

    learning encourages relational processing. As mentioned earlier, item-metho

    directed forgetting is usually greater than list-method directed forgettin

    According to our analysis, this difference in the m agnitude of directed forgettin

    should be greater when subjects study pictures or study words with image

    instructions that when they study words alone, particularly when the wor

    belong to a single category.One interpretation of directed forgetting is that contextual information abo

    D I R E C T E D F O R G E T T I N G 63

  • 8/3/2019 Basden Basden 1996

    4/23

    retrieval inhibition. Unfortunately, a potential source of confounding is present

    if subjects base their judgements of list membership on item strength. If this

    were the case, judgements of list membership might not reflect context

    information alone. A more direct measure of memory for context information is

    to have subjects indicate the approximate serial position of previously recalled

    targets. Tzeng, Lee, and Wetzel (1979) introduced this technique. With the itemmethod they found that serial position information was remembered better for

    Remember than for Forget targets. No control subjects were tested, so no

    absolute assessments were possible.

    The results of Tzeng et al. are equally consistent with the idea that Forget

    items are less extensively processed with the item method. If Remember and

    Forget targets are equally well processed with the list method, then accuracy of

    serial position information should be equivalent for the Forget and Remember

    items with this method. However, if Geiselman et al. are correct and context

    memory is reduced by directed forgetting instructions, then memory for serial

    position of F orget targets should be affected with both the item and the list

    methods.

    In this experiment we asked our subjects to indicate approximate serial

    positions for the items they recalled. To determ ine the influence of directed

    forgetting instructions on absolute memory for contextual information, we

    included control subjects who did not receive directed forgetting instructions. If

    contextual information is lost during directed forgetting, then memory for serial

    position should be less accurate for Forget targets in the experimental conditions

    than in the control conditions.

    Although control subjects were included to permit measurement of absolute

    loss of contextual informa tion, their inclusion also permitted us to com pare the

    absolute effects of directed forgetting instructions on Remember and Forget

    targets for each of the two methods. Past research has shown that directed

    forgetting instructions yield not only lower recall of Forget targets, but alsogreater recall of Remember targets (see Bjork, 1972).

    Method

    Subjects. We tested 194 students enrolled in introductory psychology

    classes at California State University, Fresno. Their participation was in partial

    fulfilment of course requirements. Approximately 22 subjects were tested in

    each of the nine treatment conditions.

    636 B A S D E N A N D B A S D E N

  • 8/3/2019 Basden Basden 1996

    5/23

    was designated Forget and Remember equally often across groups. Order w

    varied across groups to ensure that particular targets did not consistently occup

    favourable serial positions across m ethods. Subjects responded by writing the

    recall on lined pages included in their response booklets.

    Design. A 3 2 2 mixed factorial design was employed with studformat (pictures, words with imagery instructions, or words without image

    instructions) and method ( list or i tem) as between-subjects factors an

    instruction (Remember or Forget) as a within-subject factor. Three outsid

    control groups were also tested, one with each of the three study format

    Subjects in these groups were not given directed forgetting instructions.

    Procedure. Groups of up to six subjects at a time were assigned to testin

    conditions in accordance with a block-randomisation schedule. Subjects we

    given instructions appropriate to their study format prior to target presentatio

    For the image study format, subjects were instructed to form an image of eac

    word as it was presented to them. For the picture and word study format

    subjects were simply instructed to do their best to recall all targets. A blan

    screen or a spot of l ight of 1.1 second duration followed each target for a

    subjects. Subjects tested with the item method were given the followin

    additional instructions:

    After each item has been on the screen for a few seconds, it will be followed by a

    blank screen or a spot of light. A blank screen means you are to forget that item but

    a spot of light means you are to remember that item.

    Subjects tested by the list method were not told to differentiate among items an

    were told to ignore the presence or absence of lights.

    After subjects had read the initial instructions, they were shown the words pictures individually at a three-second rate. After the first 12 targets had bee

    presented, subjects tested by the item method were told to rest for a few second

    Subjects tested by the list method were given directed forgetting instructions f

    the first list. Thus, the time interval between list halves was equivalent for th

    two methods. Then the remaining 12 targets were presented.

    Target presentation was im media tely followed by a distractor task intended

    purge short-term memory. Subjects com pleted simple arithmetic problems

    their response booklets for 30 seconds; then they were instructed to write many of the targets they had studied as they could remember, including both th

    D I R E C T E D F O R G E T T I N G 63

  • 8/3/2019 Basden Basden 1996

    6/23

    through to ``6 if it occurred among the last four targets (cf Tzeng et al., 1979).

    After the experiment was over, the subjects were told the purpose of the forget

    instruction and were asked not to discuss the experiment with other potential

    subjects.

    Results

    The dependent variable in free recall was the proportion of Remember or Forget

    targets recalled. For control subjects, targets that occupied the same serial

    positions as actual Remember and Forget targets in the experimental conditions

    were designated nominal Remember and Forget targets. Thus, determination of

    nominal Remember and Forget targets for individual control subjects depended

    on whether the comparison was with list method or item method experimental

    subjects. Mean recall proportions for each of the conditions are shown in Table 1.

    A mixed three-factor analysis of variance was performed with method and

    study format as between-subjects factors and instruction as a repeated measure.

    The three-factor interaction was significant, F(2, 127) = 3.53, M Se = 0.036.

    Subsequent analysis revealed that the simple interaction between method and

    instruction was significant for pictures, F(1, 42) = 4.21, MSe = 0.032; and for

    words with images, F(1, 45) = 14.89, M Se = 0.027, but was not significant for

    words w ithout images, F

  • 8/3/2019 Basden Basden 1996

    7/23

    M Se = 0.037, for words with images, F(1, 22) = 95.86, M Se = 0.022, and f

    words without images, F(1, 19) = 15.62, M Se = 0.043. With the list m ethod, th

    magnitude of directed forgetting was also statistically significant for all thr

    study formats: for pictures, F(1, 21) = 12.47, M Se = 0.038, for words wi

    images, F(1, 23) = 11.12, MSe = 0.031, and for words without images, F(

    21) = 20.45, MSe = 0.048.

    As mentioned earlier, we compared the list method and item method directe

    forgetting subjects with the same control subjects. This necessitated separa

    three-factor analyses of variance for each method, with study format and grou

    (experimental vs. control) as between-subjects factors and instruction as

    repeated measures factor. For the item method analysis, the effect of stud

    format was significant, F(2, 120) = 7.77, MSe = 0.035. Images were recall

    better than pictures or words. The interaction between instruction and group w

    significant, F(1, 120) = 65.21. Tests of simple effects confirmed that recall

    Remember targets was greater for experimental than for control subjects, F(

    120) = 13.04, M Se = 0 .158 ; b ut rec all of Fo rg et t arg ets w a s poo re r f

    experimental than for control subjects F(1 120) = 46 06 MSe = 0 1922

    F

    FIG. 1. Magnitude of directed forgetting as a function of study format and instruction.

    D I R E C T E D F O R G E T T I N G 63

  • 8/3/2019 Basden Basden 1996

    8/23

    the list method analysis, the interaction between group type and instruction was

    again significant, F(1, 123) = 27.87. As in the analysis of the item method, recall

    of Remember targets was greater for experimental than for control subjects, F(1,

    123 ) = 8.03, M Se = 0 .2 01; bu t r eca ll of F or ge t ta rge ts w as p oo re r f or

    experimental than for control subjects, F(1, 123) = 17.75, M Se = 0.017.

    An analysis conducted on the proportion of correct serial position placementsfor the item method yielded a significant main effect for instruction, F(1,

    60) = 5.64, M Se = 0.054. The proportion of correct placements was 0.50 for

    Remember targets and 0.40 for Forget targets. A similar analysis for the list

    m ethod yielded no significant effects, the mean propor tion of c orrect placements

    was 0.42 for Remember targets and 0.48 for Forget targets.

    An analysis was conducted on the proportion of correct serial position

    placements of Forget i tems contrasting directed forgetting subjects with

    controls. Subjects placed Forget targets equally accurately in the experimental

    and control conditions, Fs < 1. This held true for both the item and list methods.

    Discussion

    W e have hypothesised that study formats encouraging item-specific processing

    would increase directed forgetting m ore with the item method than with the list

    m ethod. This hypothe sis was confirmed . The difference in magnitude of directed

    forgetting with the item and list methods was greater when subjects studied

    pictures or words for which they formed images than when they simply studied

    words. Directed forgetting was actually equivalent for the two methods when

    subjects studied words w ithout forming images. The se results are consistent with

    our distinction between item and list methods, i.e. that relational processing

    underlies directed forgetting with the list method, but that item-specific

    processing underlies directed forgetting with the item method.

    The present results are not consistent with those of Bugelski (1970), whoreported an absence of directed forgetting with the item method when subjects

    were asked to form images of the targets they studied. His report is quite sketchy

    with regard to procedures, but we assume that they differed from our own in

    some important way. Our results are consistent with those of Bray and his

    colleagues, e.g. Bray et al. (1983). These investigators reported robust directed

    forgetting when subjects studied pictures.

    Bugelski had argued, on the basis of his failure to find directed forgetting,

    that subjects could not inhibit targets for which they had formed images. On thebasis of our own results we would argue that subjects did not carry out extensive

    640 B A S D E N A N D B A S D E N

  • 8/3/2019 Basden Basden 1996

    9/23

    Remember items. In the present study this occurred with both the list and ite

    methods. Recall was poorer for Forget targets and better for Remember targe

    as compared with controls. Bjork (1970) has argued that Remember targets a

    subject to less interference in the experimental conditions. This argument cou

    be applied to either method of directed forgetting.

    If differential processing underlies item-method forgetting, then seriposition information should be better for Remember than for Forget items wi

    this method. If differential processing is not responsible for list-method directe

    forgetting, then serial position information should be the same for Rememb

    and Forget items with this method. As predicted, memory for serial positio

    information was better for Remember than for Forget i tems with the ite

    method, but not with the list method. These results support our position.

    To test the hypothesis expressed by Geiselman et al. (1983) that direct

    forgetting occurs when subjects lose access to contextual information, w

    compared accuracy of serial position information for Forget targets in th

    experimental and control conditions. Although Geiselman et al. reported th

    memory for list membership of Forget items was greater for control than fo

    experimental subjects, we found no corresponding difference in the accuracy

    memory for serial position. Furthermore, our results were quite similar with th

    list and item methods. Thus, our results do not support the hypothesis th

    directed forgetting results from loss of contextual inform ation. O f course, a qui

    different picture might emerge if recollection for serial position of all items ha

    been assessed.

    Thus, results of Experiment 1 were consistent with the hypothesis th

    retrieval inhibition and relational processing underlie directed forgetting wi

    the list method, but that differential item-specific processing underlies directe

    forgetting with the item method. In Experiment 2, we compared the influence

    the list- and item-m ethod directed forgetting on performance in an impli

    memory test. Current research and reviews (e.g. Brown & Mitchell, 199 Thapar & Greene, 1994) indicate that levels of processing manipulations ma

    influence priming on implicit tests. If, as we suggest, R emember targets a

    more extensively processed than Forget targets with the item method, then item

    method directed forgetting may occur on implicit tests but list-method direct

    forgetting should not.

    E X P E R IM E N T 2

    Directed forgetting on implicit tests has sometimes been observed with the ite

    D I R E C T E D F O R G E T T I N G 64

  • 8/3/2019 Basden Basden 1996

    10/23

    (e.g. word fragment cued recall, stem-cued recall, recognition, and word

    association) do not produce list-method directed forgetting (see Basden et al.,

    1993). List-method directed forgetting instructions appear to act on the list as a

    whole rather than on individual items.

    In previous experiments that obtained item-method directed forgetting on

    implicit tests (e.g. MacLeod, 1989), perceptual tests such as word fragmentcompletion were used. Such tests are typically less sensitive to differential

    processing (see Roediger & McDermott, 1993) than are conceptual tests (tests

    relying on meaning). Thus, demonstrations of item-method directed forgetting

    should be easier to obtain on implicit conceptual tests. In research reported

    earlier we failed to observe item-method directed forgetting with an implicit

    word association test (Basden et al., 1993, Experiments 1 & 2). However, this

    failure may have resulted from a floor effect, as priming was not statistically

    significant with the conceptual test we used. The present experiment represents

    another attempt to obtain item-method directed forgetting with an implicit

    conceptual test. The list method was included for purposes of comparison.

    After studying a list of low-frequency words with either item- or list-method

    directed forgetting instructions, the subjects were given an implicit test of

    general knowledge. Definitions of targets and distractors were provided at the

    time of the test, and subjects were instructed to respond to each of them with the

    first word that came to mind. Low-frequency words were used because such

    materials typically yield higher levels of priming than do high-frequency words

    (Roediger & McDermott, 1993). As this test, like other implicit tests, includes

    item-by-item cueing, i t should be sensit ive to differential processing of

    Reme mber and Forget items of the sort we believe the item method encourages.

    Therefore, we expected directed forgetting w ith the item method but not with the

    list method.

    After completing the implicit general knowledge test, subjects were asked to

    free recall the Remember and Forget targets. We expected directed forgettingwith the list method as well as with the item method on this test, because free

    recall is sensitive to the retrieval inhibition we believe occurs with the list

    method. We expected greater directed forgetting with the item method than with

    the list method as in our previous research.

    Method

    Subjects. We initially tested 68 subjects and later added 69 more subjects.We also tested 28 similar subjects in a pilot study used to assess the materials.

    642 B A S D E N A N D B A S D E N

  • 8/3/2019 Basden Basden 1996

    11/23

    Dictionary. The materials were then piloted to confirm that prospective subjec

    could match the words with their definitions. These definitions were used

    prepare a test of general knowledge.

    All materials were presented using a microcomputer. The subjects typed the

    responses at the computer keyboard.

    Design. We employed a mixed two-factor design with method (item or lis

    as a between-subjects factor and instruction (Forget or R emember) as a withi

    subject factor. The dependent measure was the number of experimente

    designated terms produced on the test of general knowledge and on th

    subsequent free recall test.

    Procedure. Subjects were assigned to the method of directed forgetting

    accordance with a block-randomisation schedule. After entering the conditio

    code at the computer keyboard, the experimenter left the test cubicle; th

    remainder of the procedure was automatically administered. The programm

    randomly selected a study list of 48 targets and 48 distractors from the corpus

    192 words. The 48 list members were randomly separated into 24 Rememb

    and 24 Forget targets. Individual random presentation and testing orders we

    also program-generated. Prior to list presentation all subjects w ere informed th

    their ability to remem ber list membe rs would be tested later. Subjects tested wi

    the item method were also given the following instructions:

    After each word has been on the screen for a few seconds, a brief signal will tell

    you whether to REMEMBER it or FORGET it. If the signal is REMEMBER you

    should be prepared to recall the word later, but if the signal is FORGET you can

    forget the word.

    After reading these initial instructions, all subjects were shown the targeindividually at a six-second rate. For subjects tested with the item metho

    ``REMEM BER or ``FORGET joined each item during its final three secon

    of exposure. After the first 24 targets had been presented, subjects tested wi

    the item method w ere told to rest for a few seconds and subjects tested with th

    list method were given the following directed forgetting instructions:

    The list you have just studied was only for practice. You can forget it now. The list

    you will see next is the one we want you to remember, so forget the practice listand concentrate on this new list.

    D I R E C T E D F O R G E T T I N G 64

  • 8/3/2019 Basden Basden 1996

    12/23

    thought was associated with each commodity on the screen, e.g. ``Georgia for

    PEACHES. Subjects continued with this task until they had responded to all

    commodities or until 30 seconds had elapsed without a response.

    Subjects were told that the general knowledge test, which immediately

    followed the distractor task, was part of a different experiment. The definitions

    corresponding to the 48 targets and 48 distractors were displayed individually inrandom order. The subjects were instructed to type the first appropriate w ord

    that came to mind for each definition. The next definition appeared immediately

    after the subjects response to the previous one, or after 30 seconds in any case.

    After the subjects had completed the general knowledge test, they were given

    a free recall test. Subjects typed their recall on the computer keyboard,

    continuing until 30 seconds had elapsed without additional recall.

    Results

    A preliminary analysis revealed no differences in results for the initial and

    subsequently added subjects, so the two data sets were combined for the analysis

    reported here.

    Priming Scores. The dependent variable was the proportion of designated

    terms (Remember targets, Forget targets, and distractors) that were produced in

    response to their definitions on the general know ledge test. Mean proportions foreach group are shown in Table 2.

    We performed a two-factor mixed analysis of variance on the proportions of

    target words produced on the general knowledge test. More targets were given

    with the list method than with the item method, F(1, 135) = 11.33, MSe = 0.03;

    a nd m or e ta rge ts w er e g ive n t o d efi niti ons o f R em em be r w ords th an to

    definitions of Forget words, F(1, 135) = 20.33, M Se = 0.007. The interaction was

    also significant, F(1, 135) = 6.82. Simple tests established that directed

    f or ge tt in g w a s s ig ni fi ca nt w ith t he ite m m e th od , F( 1 , 6 7 ) = 2 2 . 80 ,

    M Se = 0.008, but not with the list method, F(1, 68) = 2.02, M Se = 0.007.

    To determine whether priming had occurred, the proportions of Remember

    and Forget targets provided were individually compared with the proportion of

    T A B L E 2

    Mean Proportions of Remember, Forget, and Distractor Terms Produced on the General

    K n o w l e d g e T e s t i n E x p e r i m e n t 2

    Word Type

    644 B A S D E N A N D B A S D E N

  • 8/3/2019 Basden Basden 1996

    13/23

    d is tr a ct o rs p r ov id e d . R e m e m b e r- ta r ge t p r im i n g w a s si g ni fic a n t, F(

    135) = 239.40, M Se = 0.007. The interaction with method was significant, F(

    135) = 5.20, reflecting greater priming with the list method than with the ite

    method. Simple tests confirmed significant priming of Remember targets wi

    both the list method, F(1, 68) = 159.18, M Se = 0.007, and the item method, F(

    67) = 86.15, MSe = 0.007.The results of the Forget-target priming analysis were similar to those for th

    Remember-target priming analysis. Both the effect of method, F(1, 135) = 8.2

    M Se = 0.006, and the effect of instruction, F (1, 135) = 137.48, w ere significan

    The significant interaction, F(1, 135) = 29.47, again reflected greater primin

    with the list m ethod than with the item method. Simple tests confirmed th

    Forget-target priming was significant for both the list method, F(1,68) = 149.5

    M Se = 0.006, and the item method, F(1, 67) = 19.51. Thus, both Remember an

    Forget words showed significant priming with both methods, but priming w

    greater with the list method than with the item method.

    F r ee R e c a ll . A tw o- fa cto r m i xe d m o de l a na ly sis of v arianc e w

    performed on the proportions of Remember and Forget words produced durin

    the final free recall test. Mean recall proportions for Remember and Forg

    words were 0.14 and 0.11 with the list method, and 0.17 and 0.06 with the ite

    method. The effect of instruction w as significant, F(1,134) = 60.53, M Se = 0.00

    The interaction was also significant, F(1, 134) = 24.32, MSe = 0.005, reflectin

    greater directed forgetting with the item method than with the list metho

    Directed forgetting was significant with both the item method, F (1,67) = 75.5

    M Se = 0.006, and the list method, F(1, 67) = 4.36, MSe = 0.005.

    Discussion

    Earlier we reported our failure to obtain item-method directed forgetting on aimplicit word association test (Basden et al., 1993, Experiments 1 & 2). W

    attributed that null result to the low levels of priming that were observed. Th

    experiment was designed to provide a more adequate test of directed forgettin

    on a conceptual implicit test; i.e. one that showed adequate priming. We we

    successful in that priming on the general knowledge test was significant for bo

    Remember and Forget words and with both methods. There are several reaso

    for greater priming in the present experiment. First, we used more targets, thu

    permitting more observations. Second, the general know ledge items were relatively low frequency. Third, more redintegrative information is provided b

    D I R E C T E D F O R G E T T I N G 64

  • 8/3/2019 Basden Basden 1996

    14/23

    confirmed. Thus, i tem-method directed forgetting occurred on both the

    conceptual test and on the explicit free recall test, but list-method directed

    forgetting occurred only on the latter. These results are consistent with our

    hypotheses that (a) Remember targets are more extensively processed than

    Forget targets with the item method but not with the list method, and (b) tests of

    implicit memory are sensitive to the effects of differential processing but not to those of retrieval inhibition. When subjects are given retrieval cues on an item-

    by-item basis, suppression of the entire Forget list is unlikely to occur.

    G ol di ng , L o ng a nd M a cl eo d ( 199 4) ha ve s ug ge st ed th at, as d ir ec te d

    forgetting instructions may result in differential processing of Remember and

    Forget words, the underlying basis for directed forgetting on implicit tests may

    be differential levels of processing, much as w e have suggested here. However,

    they did not distinguish between item- and list-method directed forgetting. If

    differential processing underlies directed forgetting with both the list and item

    method, then the list method should yield directed forgetting on implicit tests.

    Not only should it occur, it should be greater than with the item method. In

    mixed-list designs, investigators have found that the levels of processing effect

    is larger when targets are blocked at presentation as in the list method rather than

    random ly intermingled as in the item method (Challis & Brodbeck, 1992;

    Thapar & Greene, 1994). The fact that this does not occur makes it clear that

    differential processing of Remember and Forget items is not responsible for list-

    method directed forgetting.

    It could be argued that directed forgetting on item-method implicit tests

    results from contamination of a supposedly pure test of automatic processing by

    intentional retrieval. Toth, Reingold, & Jacoby (1994) recently applied the

    process dissociation procedure to levels of processing findings. They reported

    that conscious recollection accounted for the advantage of semantically

    processed targets relative to nonsemantically processed targets on implicit

    tests. Previously, item-method directed forgetting has been observed whensubjects were told to engage in intentional retrieval (Basden et al., 1993,

    Experiment 3) or when the implicit test was not well camouflaged (MacLeod,

    1989). Russo and Andrade (1995) recently applied the process dissociation

    procedure to word fragment completion data that showed a directed forgetting

    effect with the inclusion test procedure. Their estimate of the contribution of

    conscious recollection was greater for Rem ember than for Forget targets. Their

    results are consistent with our argument (Basden et al., 1993) that intentional

    retrieval may contribute to directed forgetting on implicit tests. However, aninclusion test encourages subjects to consciously recollect the target items,

    646 B A S D E N A N D B A S D E N

  • 8/3/2019 Basden Basden 1996

    15/23

    taken to reduce the likelihood of contamination by intentional retrieval

    s ugg e st ed b y R oe d ig er a nd M c D e rm o tt (1 99 3) . A pp l ic at io n o f p ro ce

    dissociation procedures to properly conducted implicit tests may be gratuitou

    In Experiment 3, we provided a further test of our contention that item

    method directed forgetting involves differential processing but list-metho

    directed forgetting does not. If Remember words are more extensively processe than Forget words with the item method but not with the list method, directe

    forgetting should be absent when only the familiarity of Remember and Forg

    items is assessed.

    E X P E R IM E N T 3

    In this experiment we turned to the distinction between Know and Recolle

    judgements presented by Tulving (1985) and developed extensively by Gardin(e.g. Gardiner, 1988; Gardiner & Java, 1993).

    3Such judgements partitio

    recognit ion performance into two components, one reflecting conscio

    recollection (Recollect judgements), and the other reflecting familiarity (Kno

    judgements). Recollect and Know judgements are obtained by asking subjects

    indicate for each recognised item whether its actual occurrence in the study li

    is remembered or it merely seems familiar.

    Gardiner (1988, Experiment 1) observed a levels of processing effect wi

    Recollect judgements but not with Know judgeme nts. Gardiner, Gawlik, an Richardson-Klavehn (1994) found i tem-method directed forgetting w

    Recollect but not with Know judgements. In the present experiment w

    included both methods of directed forgett ing. If, as we have propose

    Remember and Forget targets are differentially processed with the item metho

    but not the list m ethod, then directed forgetting should occur with Recolle

    judgements for the item method but not for the list method. Neither method w

    expected to yield directed forgetting with Know judgements.

    M e t h o d .

    Subjects, Materials, and Apparatus. We tested 40 students of the sam

    description as in our earlier experiments. The word list and apparatus were th

    same as in Experiment 2.

    Design. The design w as a 2 2 factorial. Method (item or list) w

    manipulated between subjects and instructions (Remember or Forget) wmanipulated within subjects. All subjects were given a recognition judgeme

    D I R E C T E D F O R G E T T I N G 64

  • 8/3/2019 Basden Basden 1996

    16/23

    Procedure. The study instructions and method of presentation were the

    same as in Experiment 2. List presentation was immediately followed by a

    recognition judgement test. Instructions were drawn verbatim from Rajaram

    (1993). To paraph rase, subjects were told to respond w ith ``N (Not Recognised),

    if they did not remem ber having studied any given word; w ith ``K (Know ), if the

    word appeared familiar, i.e. if they we re aware of the word in the context of theexperiment, but were unable to recollect any contextual information about that

    word; and with ``R (R ecollect) if they consciously recollected having studied

    that w ord. Each target or distractor wa s presented in uppercase letters, centred on

    the monitor screen. Definitions of the available responses appeared below each

    target and distractor. Subjects pressed ``N, ``K, or ``R, on the computer

    keyboard for each word. Each new word was presented immediately after a

    response to the previous word, or after 30 seconds in any case.

    Results

    The mean proportions of Remember and Forget words judged as Recollect and

    Know are shown in Table 3, along with Recollect and Know judgements for

    distractors.

    A mixed two-factor analysis of variance was performed on the Recollect

    judgements for Remember and Forget targets. I t produced a s ignificant

    interaction between method and instruction, F(1, 38) = 11.32, M Se = 0.015.Subsequent simple tests showed directed forgetting with the item method, F(1,

    19) = 26.04, M Se = 0.014, but not with the list m ethod, F

  • 8/3/2019 Basden Basden 1996

    17/23

    significant, F(1, 38) = 11.54. The difference between Forget and Distract

    w ord s w as s ig nifi cant no t o nly w ith th e lis t m e tho d, F(1, 19) = 235.3

    M Se = 0.019, but also with the item method, F(1, 19) = 69.17, M Se = 0.028. Th

    difference between Forget and Distractor words was greater with the list metho

    than with the item methods.

    Discussion

    In earlier reports (see Basden et al., 1993; Basden et al., 1994), we demonstrate

    that directed forgetting occurs in recognition with the item method but not wi

    the list method when overall performance is examined. This experime

    c onf ir m s th is c onc lu si on , a nd de m o ns tr at es t ha t i t a pp lie s t o R e co ll e

    judgements and not to Know judgements. This dissociation supports o

    hypothesis that the processes underlying directed forgetting differ for the twmethods. The difference in correct Recollect judgements between Forget an

    Distractor items was greater with the list method than with the item method, b

    the corresponding difference between Remember and Distractor items was th

    same with the two methods. Forget words appear to receive less extensi

    processing than Remember words with the item method, but Remember an

    Forget words appear to receive equivalent processing with the list method.

    Our results with the item method replicated those of Gardiner et al. (1994

    They varied the delay of the Forget or Remember cue and found that increasede lay w as a ss oc iate d w it h a n inc rea se in K now judge m en ts fo r item

    accompanied by either cue. Increased cue delay was associated with an increa

    in Recollect judgements for Remember targets but not for Forget targets. The

    interpretation, with which we concur, was that maintenance rehearsal influenc

    familiarity, and hence, Know judgements. There is no corresponding increase

    Recollect judgements with delay, because elaborative rehearsal is necessary

    increase conscious recollection.

    The present results are consistent with evidence from Experiment 1 th

    subjects retain greater contextual information for Remember than for Forg

    words with the item method but not with the list method. Subjects were mo

    accurate in providing serial position information for Rem ember than for Forg

    targets with the item method than w ith the list method. The ability to rememb

    the serial position of targets reflects greater conscious recollection for the

    context. Similarly, Recollect judgem ents reflected memory for the specif

    context in which particular words occurred. This is consistent with our argume

    that the item method is more encouraging to differential processing than is th

    list method

    D I R E C T E D F O R G E T T I N G 64

  • 8/3/2019 Basden Basden 1996

    18/23

    presentation of items from a single category, having subjects study pictures or

    form images increased directed forgetting m ore with the item method than with

    the list method. In Experiment 2, directed forgetting occurred with the item

    method but not with the list method when responses to general knowledge

    questions were analysed. In Experiment 3, Recollect judgements showed

    directed forgetting w ith the item method but not with the list method. Thus, the present research further confirms that the processes underlying item- and list-

    method directed forgetting differ.

    The present results should be of interest to those who study inhibitory

    mechanisms in recall. Although directed forgetting was greater with the item

    method than with the list method when pictures and images (which encourage

    item-specific processing) w ere studied, it was equivalent with the two methods

    when words alone (which encourage relational processing) were studied. Thus,

    for materials that encourage relational processing, the retrieval inhibition

    process may yield substantial directed forgetting. Perhaps because directed

    forgetting tends to be more reliably obtained with the item method than with

    the list method (e.g. Basden et al., 1993), many recent experiments have been

    conducted with the item method, particularly when implicit tests were used

    (e.g. Gardiner et al., 1994; Golding et al., 1994; MacLeod, 1989; Russo &

    Andrade, 1995). This seems an unfortunate choice because, by our account, it

    is the list method that reflects subjects ability to lose or regain access to

    previously stored information. Understanding of retrieval inhibition in the

    context of directed forgetting may facilitate our understanding of retrieval

    inhibition in more exotic contexts, such as recovery of repressed memories

    (e.g. Cloitre et al., 1995).

    Interest in inhibitory mechanisms in memory has enjoyed a recent resurgence

    and new theoretical approaches have emerged. Zacks, Radvansky, and Hasher

    (1995) presented an approach that distinguishes betw een two different inhibitory

    mechanisms in directed forgetting. The first involves retrieval inhibition; thesecond involves the stopping of rehearsal following presentation of a Forget cue.

    The latter mechanism is one that prevents information from entering working

    memory (see Hasher & Zacks, 1988). According to this formulation older

    subjects are deficient in inhibitory processing. Target information that is

    designated as to-be-forgotten may not be successfully inhibited, either because

    subjects cannot successfully inhibit retrieval of previously stored information or

    because items designated as to-be-forgotten are processed just as effectively as

    items that are not designated as to-be-forgotten. Experiments conducted byZacks et al. showed that older subjects were less successful in meeting the

    650 B A S D E N A N D B A S D E N

  • 8/3/2019 Basden Basden 1996

    19/23

    It has been proposed (Geiselman et al., 1983, p.63) that directed forgettin

    can be accounted for in terms of a mechanism by which ``an F cue serves

    initiate a process that inhibits the accessibility of a space in time in episod

    memory. However, when we measured memory for context by asking subjec

    to provide serial position information for the Forget items they had recalled, w

    found that experimental subjects were as accurate in their judgements as wecontrol subjects. As our measure of contextual memory is less likely to hav

    been contaminated by attributional errors than is the measure used by Geiselma

    et al., we conclude that there is little evidence to support an explanation

    retrieval inhibition in directed forgetting that relies on loss of contextu

    information.

    In conclusion, our results clearly show the desirability of distinguishin

    between the two methods of directed forgetting, as initially proposed by Bjor

    (1972). Item-method directed forgetting relies on processing distinctive featur

    of individual targets and influences retention of Remember and Forget targets

    a manner analogous to levels of processing procedures. On the other hand, lis

    method directed forgetting encourages relational processing and appears

    differentially influence performance only on a free recall test, a test that

    sensitive to relational processing. When subjects are provided with retriev

    information in the form of word associates, word fragments, or copy cue

    directed forgetting with the list method disappears. Only when subjects a

    required to rely on their own organisational system is list-method directe

    forgetting manifested. These observations support the idea that list-metho

    directed forgetting is dependent on organisational (relational) processin

    whereas item-method directed forgetting is dependent on distinctive (item

    specific) processing.

    Manuscript received 25 May 19

    Manuscript accepted 11 December 19

    R E F E R E N C E S

    Basden, B.H., Basden, D.R., Coe, W.C., Decker, S., & Crutcher, K. (1994). Retrieval inhibition

    directed forgetting and posthypnotic amnesia. International Journal of Clinical and Experimen

    Hypnosis, 42, 184203.

    Basden, B.H., Basden, D.R., & Gargano, G.J. (1993). Directed forgetting in implicit and expli

    memory tests: A comparison of methods. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learnin

    Memory and Cognition , 19, 603616.

    Bjork, R.A. (1970). Positive forgetting: The noninterference of items intentionally forgotte

    D I R E C T E D F O R G E T T I N G 65

  • 8/3/2019 Basden Basden 1996

    20/23

    Bray, N.W ., Justice, E.M., & Zahm, D.N. (1983). Two developmental transitions in selective

    remembering strategies. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 36, 4355.

    Brown, A.S., & Mitchell, D.B. (1994). A re-evaluation of semantic versus nonsemantic processing

    in implicit memory. Memory & Cognition, 22 , 533541.

    Bugelski, B.R. (1970). Words and things and images. American Psychologist, 25 , 10021012.

    Challis, B.H., & Brodbeck, D.R. (1992). Level of processing affects priming in word fragment

    completion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , 18 , 595607.

    Cloitre, M., Cancienna, J., Brodsky, B., Zeitlin, S.B., Dulit, R., & Perry, S. (1996). Memory

    performance among women with parental abuse histories: Enhanced directed forgetting or

    directed remembering? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105 , 204211.

    Einstein, G.O., & Hunt, R.R. (1980). Levels of processing and organization: Additive effects of

    individual-item and relational processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning

    and M emory, 6, 588598.

    Epstein, W. (1970). Facilitation of retrieval resulting from p ost-input exclusion of part of the input.

    Journal of Experimental Psychology, 86, 190195.Gardiner, J.M . (1988). Functional aspects of recollective experience. Memory & Cognition, 16,

    309313.

    Gardiner, J.M ., Gawlik, G., & Richardson-Klavehn, A. (1994). M aintenance rehearsal affects

    knowing not remembering; Elaborative rehearsal affects remembering not knowing. Psycho-

    nomic Bulletin and Review, 1, 107110.

    Gardiner, J.M., & Java, R.I. (1993). Recognizing and remembering. In A. Collins, S. Gathercole, M.

    Conway, & P. Morris (Eds.), Theories of memory (pp.163188). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence

    Erlbaum Associates Inc.

    Geiselman, R.E., & Bagheri, B. (1985). R epetition effects in directed forgetting: Evidence for

    retrieval inhibition. Memory & Cognition, 13 , 5762.

    Geiselman, R.E., Bjork, R.A., & Fishman, D.L. (1983). Disrupted retrieval in directed forgetting: A

    link with posthypnotic amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 112, 5872.

    Golding, J.M ., Long, D.L., & MacLeod, C.M. (1994). You cant always forget what you want:

    Directed forgetting of related words. Journal of Memory and Language , 33 , 493510.

    Hasher, L., & Zacks, R.T. (1988). Working memory, comprehension, and aging: A review and a

    new view. In G.H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 22, pp.193

    225). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Hunt, R.R., & Einstein, G.O. (1981). Relational and item-specific information in mem ory. Journal

    of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20 , 497514.Hunt, R.R., & Marschark, M. (1989). Yet another picture of imagery: The roles of shared and

    distinctive information in memory. In M.A. McDaniel & M. Pressley (Eds.), Imagery and related

    mnemonic processes: Theories, individual differences, and applications. New York: Springer-

    Verlag.

    Hunt, R.R., & McDaniel, M.A. (1993). The enigma of organization and distinctiveness. Journal of

    Memory and Language, 32 , 421445.

    Johnston, H.M . (1994). Processes of successful intentional forgetting. Psychological Bulletin, 116,

    274292.

    MacLeod, C.M. (1989). Directed forgetting affects both direct and indirect tests of memory.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition , 15 , 1321.

    Paller K A (1990) Recall and stem-completion priming have different electrophysiological

    652 B A S D E N A N D B A S D E N

  • 8/3/2019 Basden Basden 1996

    21/23

    Roediger, H.L. III, & McDermott, K.B. (1993). Implicit memory in normal human subjects. In

    Spinnler & F. Boller (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology, Vol. 8 (pp.63131). Amsterda

    Elsevier.

    Russo, R., & Andrade, J. (1995). The directed forgetting effect in word fragment completion: A

    application of the process dissociation procedure. The Quarterly Journal of Experimen

    Psychology: Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 48A, 405423.

    Snodgrass, J.G., & Vanderwart, M. (1980). A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for namagreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of Experimen

    Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 174215.

    Thapar, A., & Greene, R.L. (1994). Effects of level of processing on implicit and explicit tas

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20 , 671679.

    Toth, J.P., Reingold, E.M., & Jacoby, L.L. (1994). Toward a redefinition of implicit memo

    Process dissociations following elaborative processing and self-generation. Journal

    Experimental Psychology: Learning, M emory, and Cognition, 20 , 290303.

    Tulving, E. (1985). Memory and consciousness. Canadian Psychologist, 26, 112.

    Tulving, E., Schacter, D.L., & Stark, H.A. (1982). Priming effects in word-fragment completion independent of recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memo

    and Cognition, 8, 336342.

    T zeng, O.J.L ., L ee, A.T ., & Wetzel, C .D. (1979). T emporal coding in verbal informat

    processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 5, 5264.

    Woodward, A.E., B jork, R.A., & Jongeward, R.H. (1973). Recall and recognition as a function

    primary rehearsal. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12 , 608617.

    Zacks, R .T., Radvansky, G., & Hasher, L. (1996). Studies of directed forgetting in older adu

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 22 , 143156.

    D I R E C T E D F O R G E T T I N G 65

  • 8/3/2019 Basden Basden 1996

    22/23

  • 8/3/2019 Basden Basden 1996

    23/23